The scheme of
correspondence and its role in children’s mathematics
Terezinha Nunes*, Peter
Bryant, Deborah Evans and Daniel Bell
Background.
There are two theories
about the origin of children’s understanding of
multiplicative
reasoning. One is that multiplicative reasoning has its origin in
repeated
addition. The other is
that children’s scheme of one-to-many correspondence is the
origin of their
multiplicative reasoning, while addition and subtraction originate
from
the schemes of joining
and separating.
Aim.
The aim of this paper
was to assess the evidence for these two theories and
provide new relevant
evidence.
Sample.
Two studies were
carried out with children from state-supported schools
that served a varied
constituency with respect to socio-economic background. Neither
sample had been taught
about multiplication or division in school.
Method.
In the first study,
the children’s progress in multiplicative reasoning was
assessed after one
group received instruction on one-to-many correspondences and
the other on repeated
addition. In the second study, the intervention group was shown
how to use
correspondences to solve multiplicative reasoning problems and the
control group solved
visual non-numerical problems.
Results.
In the first study,
with children aged 6–7 years, the correspondence group
made significantly
more progress in multiplicative reasoning problems than the
repeated addition
group. The second study showed that it is possible to teach young
children (aged 4–5
years) to solve multiplicative reasoning problems. In both studies,
additive or
multiplicative reasoning in the pre-test was a specific predictor of
post-test
performance on the same
type of task.
Conclusions.
The results
support the theory that the scheme of one-to-many
correspondences is the
origin of children’s multiplicative reasoning. This finding has
important educational
implications.
Cardinal numbers are
used in primary school mathematics to represent two things:
quantities and
relations. Thompson (1993) suggested that ‘a person constitutes a
quantity by conceiving
of a quality of an object in such a way that he or she understands
the possibility of
measuring it. Quantities, when measured, have numerical value, but we
need not measure them
or know their measures to reason about them.’ (pp. 165–166).
So if we think that we
could count the number of children in the class who like
mathematics, we would
be establishing the quantity ‘children who like maths’. If we say
‘12 children in this
class like maths’, the number 12 represents this quantity.
We also use numbers to
represent relations between quantities: if we say that ‘there
are 2 more girls than
boys in the classroom’, the number two refers to a relation
between two quantities.
We could also say that there are two fewer boys than girls in the
classroom. The two
sentences express the same relation between these two quantities;
one is simply expressed
as the converse of the other.
It is undoubtedly
important that children understand quantities and how numbers
represent them. But
this is not sufficient for learning mathematics. Mathematics is a
science of relations.
According to Thompson (1993), ‘Quantitative reasoning is the
analysis of a situation
into a quantitative structure – a network of quantities and
quantitative
relationships … A prominent characteristic of reasoning
quantitatively is
that numbers and
numeric relationships are of secondary importance, and do not enter
into the primary
analysis of a situation. What is important is relationships among
quantities’ (p. 165).
This paper analyses the
role that the scheme of correspondence plays in children’s
understanding of
relations between quantities. No one would dispute the claim that
one-to-one
correspondence is a sine qua non for counting, and philosophers as
well as
researchers of
children’s development have suggested that knowledge of correspon-
dence relations is at
the root of understanding equivalences between sets in the absence
of counting (Decock,
2008; Frydman & Bryant, 1988; Piaget, 1952a). The focus in this
paper is on the scheme
of one-to-many correspondence and the role that it plays in
children’s
understanding of multiplicative relations. We argue that the scheme
of
correspondence provides
children with a natural and effective foundation for
understanding relations
between quantities, and that schools could build on this
scheme to advance
children’s awareness of the very notion of modelling the world
through mathematics.
In the first section,
we review briefly evidence that children use their informally
learned knowledge of
one-to-many correspondence to solve multiplication and division
problems before they
start to be taught about multiplication and division in school.
The knowledge of how to
use one-to-many correspondences to solve multiplication and
division problems is
informal in the sense that it can be learned outside school (for a
definition of informal
knowledge in this sense, see Ginsburg, 1982). In the second
section, we present two
empirical studies which show that it is possible to improve
children’s ability to
solve multiplication and division problems by teaching them to use
one-to-many
correspondence effectively. We suggest that this form of teaching is
effective in brief
interventions because it builds on children’s informal knowledge.
In the
final section, we
discuss the implications of these results for further research and
for
education.
The scheme of
one-to-many correspondence and multiplicative reasoning
In the last two
decades, mathematics educators (e.g. Confrey, 1994; Confrey &
Harel,
1994; Steffe, 1994;
Thompson, 1994; Vergnaud, 1982, 1983) have described problems
that are solved by
addition and subtraction as ‘additive reasoning’ and problems
that are
solved by
multiplication and division as ‘multiplicative reasoning’
problems.
This categorization
focuses on the nature of the relations between quantities rather
Q1 than the specific
arithmetic operations that are used to solve problems. It also
reflects
the view that the links
between addition and subtraction, on the one hand, and between
multiplication and
division, on the other, are conceptual and based on the relations
between quantities. In
contrast, the links between addition and multiplication and those
between subtraction and
division are procedural: you can multiply by carrying out
repeated additions and
divide by using repeated subtractions.
The distinction between
additive and multiplicative reasoning raises the question of
what is the origin of
multiplicative reasoning. Fischbein, Deri, Nello, and Marino (1985)
proposed that children
use an intuitive implicit model for each arithmetical operation,
and that the model that
they adopt to help them understand multiplication is repeated
addition. This idea has
influenced a large number of studies (e.g. Bell, Greer, Grimison, &
Mangan, 1989; Clark &
Kamii, 1996; Mulligan & Mitchelmore, 1997) in which the
researchers distinguish
between repeated addition and multiplication but nevertheless
hypothesize that the
origin of multiplicative reasoning is repeated addition.
The view that
multiplicative reasoning stems from repeated addition has been
influential not only
in research but also in educational practice. Recommendations are
often made that
children should be taught about multiplication starting from the idea
of
repeated addition (e.g.
The National Strategies, 2009).
Three types of evidence
have been advanced to support the claim that the origin of
children’s
understanding of multiplication is in repeated addition. First, when
children
are asked to make up
stories that would be solved by a multiplication operation (e.g.
3 £ 5), they create
repeated addition stories more frequently than other types of stories
(Brown, 1981). The
second type of evidence is indirect: it is assumed that children hold
repeated addition as
the implicit model of multiplication because they believe that
multiplication always
‘makes bigger’ (e.g. Greer, 1992), a belief that leads them
astray
when they have to
multiply a number by a fraction (e.g. 10 £ 0:5). The third type of
evidence is that
children’s strategies for solving multiplication problems can be
described as ‘repeated
addition’ (e.g. Anghileri, 1989). However, all three of these
observations are about
responses that could result from the children’s school learning.
In all these studies,
the children had been taught about multiplication as repeated
addition. We cannot be
certain that repeated addition is the implicit model that they
bring to school from
their everyday experiences. The use of repeated addition as a
problem-solving
strategy may be a consequence of the procedural connections, which
are encouraged by
teachers, and which children form between repeated addition and
multiplication, rather
than a demonstration that the natural origin of multiplicative
reasoning is in
repeated addition. So, the evidence for the claim that children’s
implicit
model of multiplication
is repeated addition is questionable, despite its undoubted
influence. At best,
the evidence suggests that children who are taught about
multiplication as
repeated addition use this procedure to solve multiplication problems
and, perhaps from this
practice, draw the inference that multiplication makes bigger.
There is an alternative
view of the origins of children’s multiplicative reasoning.
It is that their
understanding of multiplication is founded, not on addition, but on
one-
to-many correspondence.
This view rests on the analysis of the relations that define
additive and
multiplicative reasoning and also on children’s actions when
solving
multiplicative
reasoning problems before they receive instruction in school.
Additive
reasoning involves
understanding part–whole relations. In additive reasoning problems,
one may be asked to find
a whole given the parts (a þ b ¼ ?), find a part given the whole
and the other part (a þ
? ¼ b), or find the difference between two wholes (in
comparison problems).
The whole may be constituted by static parts (e.g. five boys and
seven girls in the
class) or by a transformation (e.g. Connie had five marbles and won
seven marbles in a
game; how many marbles does she have now?). In contrast,
multiplicative
reasoning is based on the idea of an invariant relationship between
two
quantities. This
constant relation is called a ratio and can be symbolized as x ¼ f ð
yÞ.
This hypothesis,
originally proposed by Piaget (1952a) and developed later by
Vergnaud (1983) and by
Nunes and Bryant (1996), is supported also by some researchers
in mathematics
education (e.g. Thompson, 1994) and professional mathematicians
(Devlin, 2008a,b).
These authors recognize the procedural connections between
addition and
multiplication, but do not regard them as conceptual connections.
To paraphrase Devlin
(2008a), you can walk to work or go by car and you will get to
the same place; but
this does not mean that the two are the same thing.
Piaget (1952a)
pioneered research on how the scheme of one-to-many
correspondence helps
children deal with multiplicative reasoning problems. His
studies investigated
children’s understanding of multiplicative equivalences. He asked
children to put one red
flower in each of a set of vases; after these were removed,
the children placed one
blue flower in each vase. All the flowers were then removed
and only the vases were
left on the table; the children were asked to take from a box
the right number of
tubes to place one flower in each tube. Piaget reports that many
5-year-olds succeeded
in constructing the set of tubes with the same number as the
flowers by placing two
tubes in correspondence with each vase. This success, Piaget
argued, was due to the
children’s understanding that if set A (flowers) has a 2:1 ratio
to set B (vases), and
set C (tubes) also has a 2:1 ratio to set B, then A and C are
equivalent.
Since Piaget’s
pioneering work, other researchers have shown that children can
construct equivalent
sets in sharing when they need to take ratio into account. Frydman
and Bryant (1988, 1994)
asked children to share sweets fairly to two different recipients;
the size of the units
that each recipient was to be given was different – for example,
one
recipient received
units that were three times the size of the units given to the other.
The children succeeded
in constructing fair shares by using one-to-many correspon-
dences in the sharing
procedure: for each treble unit they gave to recipient A, they gave
three singles to
recipient B.
Later research showed
that children successfully use the one-to-many correspon-
dence scheme to solve
multiplicative reasoning problems before they had been taught
about multiplication
and division in school. Kouba (1989) asked young children in the
USA to solve
multiplicative reasoning problems such as: at a party, there were six
cups
and five marshmallows
in each cup; how many marshmallows were there? In a series of
problems, children were
asked to supply a missing piece of information, which could be
the product (in this
case, the total number of marshmallows) or either of the factors (the
number of groups or the
number of elements in the group).
For the children in
first and second grade, who had not received instruction on
multiplication and
division, the most important factor in predicting the children’s
solutions was which
quantity was unknown: the product, the number of groups, or the
number of elements in
the group. For example, in the problem above, about the six cups
with five marshmallows
in each cup, when the size of the groups was known (i.e. the
number of marshmallows
in each cup), the children used correspondence strategies:
they paired objects (or
tallies to represent the objects) to something that represented
the cups and counted or
added, creating one-to-many correspondences between the
cups and the
marshmallows. If they needed to find the total number of
marshmallows,
they pointed five
times to a cup (or its representation) and counted to five, paused,
and then counted from 6
to 10 as they pointed to the second ‘cup’, until they reached
the solution.
Alternatively, they may have added the number of marshmallows as they
pointed to the ‘cup’.
In contrast, when the
number of elements in each group was not known, the
children used dealing
strategies: they shared out one marshmallow (or its
representation) to each
cup, and then another, until they reached the end, and then
counted the number in
each cup. Although these actions look quite different, their aims
are the same: to
establish one-to-many correspondences between the marshmallows and
the cups.
Kouba’s description
of the children’s strategies makes it clear that the method used
for quantifying the
response could be counting or adding. The scheme of
correspondence provided
the model for the problem solution: the children counted
or added the number of
marshmallows as they pointed to each cup. Addition as a form
of counting was
observed only when the size of the groups was known: when it was
not, the children used
sharing or dealing, which established the correspondence
relations, and then
counted the items in each group to determine the value of the
quantities. Kouba’s
description leaves it clear that correspondences were used to
represent the
relations; counting or addition was procedures used to determine the
quantities.
Kouba observed that 43%
of the strategies used by the children, including first,
second, and third
graders, were appropriate. Most of the appropriate strategies adopted
by the first and
second grade children were based on correspondences: they tended to
use either direct or
partial representations (i.e. tallies for one variable and counting
or
adding for the other);
few used recall of multiplication facts. The recall of number facts
was significantly
higher among children in the third grade, who had been taught about
this way of solving
problems.
The level of success
observed by Kouba among children who had not yet received
instruction was modest
in comparison to that observed in two subsequent studies,
where the ratios were
easier. Becker (1993) asked kindergarten children in the USA,
aged 4–5 years, to
solve problems in which the correspondences were 2:1 or 3:1. The
children were more
successful with 2:1 than 3:1 correspondences, a result also reported
by Frydman and Bryant
(1988) and Piaget (1952a). The level of success improved with
age. The overall level
of correct responses achieved by 5-year-olds was 81%. This is
clearly a high level of
success for children who had not received any instruction on
multiplicative
reasoning and who were just starting to learn about addition and
subtraction at school.
Carpenter, Ansell, Franke, Fennema, and Weisbeck (1993) also
gave multiplicative
reasoning problems to US kindergarten children involving
correspondences of 2:1,
3:1, and 4:1. They observed 71% correct responses to these
problems.
These success rates
leave no doubt that many young children start school with some
understanding of
one-to-many correspondence, which they can use to learn to solve
multiplicative
reasoning problems in school. The results do not imply that children
who
use one-to-many
correspondence to solve multiplicative reasoning problems
consciously recognize
that in a multiplicative situation there is a fixed ratio linking
the two variables.
Their actions maintain a fixed ratio between the quantities but it
is
most likely that this
invariance remains, in Vergnaud’s (1997) terminology, as a ‘theorem
in action’.
In summary, there has
been for some time a theory that suggests that repeated
addition is the
implicit model that children use to make sense of the operation of
multiplication: so
repeated addition would be the origin of children’s understanding
of multiplicative
relations. Although the influence of this model both in research and
in practice is
considerable, the evidence for it is ambiguous. Most of it comes from
studies of children who
have already received instruction about multiplication as
repeated addition in
school: thus the children might have developed this conception
of multiplication as
repeated addition as a consequence of the school instruction
that they received. An
alternative view is that children’s implicit model for
multiplicative
relations is one-to-many correspondence. Children use one-to-many
correspondences to
solve multiplicative reasoning problems before they are taught
about multiplication
and division in school. They are in fact developing their
understanding of
additive and multiplicative relations at the same time when they
start school.
The effect of learning
to use the scheme of one-to-many correspondence on solving
multiplicative
reasoning problems
We report here two
studies in which we analysed children’s improvement in solving
multiplicative
reasoning problems after being taught how to use the scheme of
one-to-
many correspondence.
The first study (Park
& Nunes, 2001) compared the effectiveness of teaching children
(aged between 6 and 7
years) about repeated addition with teaching them about
correspondences in
promoting their later success in multiplicative reasoning problems.
This study has been
published but we present here some additional information not
included in its
previous publication. The second study aimed at assessing whether it
is
possible to improve
young children’s performance in multiplicative reasoning when
they are in their first
year in school (aged 4–5 years), before they have made much
progress in computation
using addition.
STUDY 1
Park and Nunes (2001)
reasoned that, if the scheme of one-to-many correspondence
offers children an
insight into multiplicative relations whereas repeated addition only
provides them with a
procedure to find a quantity, children taught to solve
multiplication problems
by using correspondences between two variables would
improve more in solving
multiplicative reasoning problems than other children, taught
about multiplication as
repeated addition. Children taught about correspondences
would have the
opportunity to construct a model of the relations between variables,
which could be used
flexibly in solving multiplicative reasoning problems. Children
taught to solve
repeated addition problems would be essentially using addition as a
procedure to determine
a total quantity but would not have the opportunity to develop
an implicit model of a
fixed ratio between two variables.
Participants
The children (N ¼ 42)
in this study were in Year 2 in two different schools in
Buckinghamshire (UK);
their mean age was 6 years 7 months. According to their
teachers, they had
received instruction on addition and subtraction in school but not
yet
on multiplication and
division.
Design
The children were given
a pre-test and a post-test, in which they were asked to solve
additive and
multiplicative reasoning problems. Between the pre- and post-tests,
they
participated in a brief
intervention, during which they solved multiplicative reasoning
problems with the
support of the researcher. They were randomly assigned to either a
repeated addition group
or to a one-to-many correspondence group. During the
intervention, the
children in the two groups were given the same number of problems
(16) with the same
numerical descriptions. However, the children in the repeated
addition group were
asked to solve one-variable problems about joining sets, whereas
those in the
one-to-many correspondence group solved problems that involved two
quantities related by a
fixed ratio. For example, the children in the repeated addition
intervention were
presented with this problem: ‘Tom has 3 toy cars. Anne has 3 dolls.
How many toys do they
have altogether?’. There is one total quantity in this problem,
number of toys, and two
parts to it, number of cars and number of dolls. In contrast, the
one-to-many
correspondence problem of the same numerical description (2 £ 3)
was:
‘Amy’s Mum is
making two pots of tomato soup. She wants to put three tomatoes in
each
pot of soup. How many
tomatoes does she need?’ This problem involves two variables,
number of tomatoes and
number of pots of soup, which are related by a fixed ratio:
three tomatoes for one
pot of soup.
Procedure
The intervention was
carried out in two sessions. In the first session, the problems
were presented to the
children orally and with the support of a picture. Figure 1 shows
the relevant picture
for the examples above. In the second session, the children had
fewer pictorial cues:
for example, instead of there being three bananas on a page, the
number 3 would be next
to a banana. Throughout the two sessions, the children were
given blocks that they
could use to solve the problems, if they found that they needed
to count.
Figure 1. One example
of parallel items presented to the correspondence and to repeated
addition
groups. (a)
Correspondence group. The picture in the booklet was shown to the
child. The experimenter
said: ‘Amy’s Mum is
making two pots of tomato soup. She wants to put three tomatoes in
each pot of
soup. How many tomatoes
does she need?’; (b) Repeated addition group. The picture in the
booklet was
shown to the child. The
experimenter said: ‘Tom has three toy cars. Anne has three dolls.
How many
toys do they have
together?
.
The problems used
during the intervention always included information about the
unit ratio: in the
example above, Amy’s Mum used three tomatoes for each pot of soup
that she made. In the
pre- and post-tests, two of the problems did not involve the unit
ratio: for example, the
children were asked: ‘Two cupcakes cost 8 pence. You buy
4 cupcakes. How much do
you have to pay?’. The inclusion of problems without the unit
ratio was considered an
important test of the flexibility of the children’s use of
relational
reasoning beyond what
they had been taught during the intervention. The problem can
be solved by repeated
addition but it is known that children make more mistakes in
problems that do not
present the unit ratio than in those in which the unit ratio is
explicit (Hart, 1981;
Ricco, 1982).
When the children were
not able to solve a problem during the intervention, the
researcher helped them
use the blocks to represent the quantities and then asked them
to solve the problem
(for the details, see Park & Nunes, 2001).
Results
At pre-test, the mean
number of correct responses (out of 8) in the addition problems
was 3.74, SD ¼ 2:51,
and in the multiplication problems it was 2.95, SD ¼ 2:18. The
distribution of scores
in both sets of problems was not skewed. The difference between
the means for the
addition and multiplication problems was statistically significant,
according to a t test
for correlated samples (t ¼ 2:75, df ¼ 41, p , :01). These pre-test
results demonstrate two
things. First, there were no floor or ceiling effects in either set
of problems. Thus, the
children had not received instruction on multiplication but
demonstrated some
competence in solving multiplication problems. This competence
is likely to be
acquired through informal learning, as they had not received school
instruction about
multiplication, and we can hypothesize that pre-test performance will
be a strong predictor
of post-test performance. Second, their performance was higher in
the addition problems,
about which they had received instruction in school.
However, there was
still room for much improvement, as shown by the mean and
standard deviation
observed at pre-test in the addition problems and by the absence of a
ceiling effect.
It was predicted that
both groups would make significant progress from pre- to
post-tests, but that
the children in the correspondence intervention group would make
more progress in the
multiplicative reasoning problems than those in the repeated
addition group. In
contrast, the children in the correspondence group were expected to
make less progress in
additive reasoning problems than the repeated addition group.
The first prediction
was supported by a significant difference between the groups in
the post-test as a
function of type of intervention. In the post-test, the mean number
of
correct responses to
multiplicative reasoning problems for the children in the
correspondence
intervention group was 6.05 (SD ¼ 1:56) and for the children in the
repeated addition group
the mean was 3.76 (SD ¼ 2:34). Both groups made some
progress from pre- to
post-tests but the children in the correspondence intervention
group made significantly
more progress in multiplicative reasoning problems than those
in the repeated
addition group. This difference supports the hypothesis that
correspondence
reasoning is a stronger basis for children to learn about
multiplicative
reasoning than repeated
addition. The stringent controls used in this design (random
assignment to the
groups, same number of problems during the intervention) give us
confidence in this
conclusion.
Contrary to our
expectation, the two groups made similar amounts of progress in
additive reasoning
problems: both performed significantly better at post-test than they
had performed at
pre-test, and the difference between the groups in the additive
problems was not
significant. It is possible that the progress that the children in
the
correspondence group
made through the intervention was not specific to using
the joining and
separating schemes: during the intervention both groups had the
experience of
representing quantities with blocks and using them to find the
answers.
This experience could
have enhanced the children’s general problem-solving skills and,
consequently, led them
to use more effectively additive reasoning schemes, which they
grasped better than the
correspondence scheme at pre-test, perhaps as a result of their
previous school
instruction. This hypothesis about developing general numerical
skills
would also explain the
progress that the children in the repeated addition group made in
multiplicative
reasoning problems; they performed better at post-test through a more
efficient use of their
general numerical skills. Their grasp of correspondences was weak
at pre-test and did not
change as a consequence of the intervention.
Two further analyses,
carried out for this paper, supported the idea that children’s
grasp of
correspondences has a specific impact on multiplicative reasoning
problems.
The first was an
analysis of the children’s progress in problems that did not
include the
unit ratio and the
second was an analysis of the partial correlations between pre- and
post-tests performance.
The first analysis
showed significant differences between the repeated addition and
the correspondence
group in problems that did not make the unit ratio explicit. The
groups did not differ
significantly at pre-test: their pre-test means (out of 2) were 0.43
and 0.52 problems
correct, respectively. At post-test, the repeated addition group
obtained a mean of 0.62
and the correspondence group a mean of 1.14 correct. About
half of the children in
the correspondence group who had answered problems without
the unit ratio
incorrectly at pre-test solved them correctly at post-test. In the
repeated
addition group, only
15% of the children made progress from pre- to post-test on
these items.
This result suggests
that the children in the repeated addition group continued to use
additive reasoning when
asked to solve multiplicative reasoning problems after their
intervention. In
contrast, the children in the correspondence group seemed to be able
to use a different
model, based on correspondences, which was more appropriate for
the relations involved
in these more complex problems.
In the second analysis,
we expected to show, through partial correlations between
additive and
multiplicative problems, that additive reasoning is distinct from
multiplicative
reasoning. Our assumption, in these partial correlations, was that
variance shared in
solutions to additive and multiplicative reasoning problems gives us
a measure of the
children’s general numerical skills. We predicted that the partial
correlation between the
pre- and post-tests addition tasks would remain significant
after controlling for
performance on the pre-test multiplication task. This would
demonstrate a specific
connection between additive reasoning measures after
partialling out these
general numerical skills. For the same reasons, we also predicted
that the partial
correlation between the pre- and post-tests multiplication tasks
would
remain significant
after controlling for performance on the pre-test addition task.
This would demonstrate
a specific connection between multiplicative reasoning
measures after
partialling out general numerical skills. However, we predicted that
the
partial correlations
between performance on the pre-test addition and post-test
multiplication tasks
would not be significant after partialling out the effects of
pre-test
multiplication
performance. Analogously, the correlation between pre-test multipli-
cation and post-test
addition tasks was expected to be non-significant after controlling
for pre-test
multiplication performance.
In summary, we expected
a significant partial correlation within problem type but a
non-significant
partial correlation between problem types. This was the pattern that
actually emerged from
the analysis. Table 1 displays these results.
Table 1. Bivariate and
partial correlations between performance on the pre- and post-tests
(N ¼ 42)
Bivariate correlations
Pre-test addition
Pre-test multiplication
Post-test addition
Partial correlations
controlling for
pre-test multiplication
Pre-test addition
Partial correlations
controlling for
pre-test addition
Pre-test multiplication
*
Pre-test multiplication
Post-test addition Post-test multiplication
.70*
.76*
53*
.41*
.71*
.30
.65*
2.16
.01
.65*
Correlations significant
at p , :01:
These results support
the idea that there are general cognitive and numerical skills
that influence
performance both in additive and multiplicative reasoning tasks but
that
there is also a specific
component in multiplicative reasoning, which is independent of
additive reasoning and
vice versa. Children’s informal knowledge of multiplication at
pre-test was a specific
predictor of their post-test performance in multiplicative
reasoning problems;
children’s pre-test performance on the additive reasoning
problems was also a
specific predictor of their performance in the additive reasoning
problems at post-test.
STUDY 2
Our second study
investigated whether it is possible to improve younger children’s
use
of correspondence
reasoning to solve multiplicative relations problems.
Participants
The children (N ¼ 32)
were recruited from two schools in Oxford and were in their first
year in school; their
mean age was 5 years 4 months (range 4 years 8 months to 5 years
9 months). They had
received no teaching about multiplication and division in school.
Design
The children were
randomly assigned either to a training group, which received
instruction on
multiplicative reasoning, or to a control group, which was taught how
to
solve visual analysis
problems. These latter problems were similar in structure to the
items used in the
British Abilities Scale-II (BAS-II) matrices subtest. Our aims in
offering
this training to the
control group were: (1) to expose the control group to the same
number of one-to-one
sessions with the researcher as the experimental group; (2) to
work with them on
non-numerical, reasoning problems, in view of our finding in Study 1
that children can
improve in a set of non-trained problems just from learning how to
use
blocks to represent
stories as a way to solve the problems; and (3) to assess whether the
control group
maintained a comparable level of motivation during their own
training.
Our predictions were:
(1) that the experimental group would make significantly
more progress in
multiplicative reasoning problems than the control group due to their
specific experience
with one-to-many correspondence during training; (2) that the
experimental group
would also make significantly more progress than the control group
in additive reasoning
problems due to the general numerical skill that they would
develop during the
training; and (3) that the children in the control group would make
significantly more
progress in the BAS-II matrices subtest than the experimental group,
due to their training
in visual analysis.
The children were
given a pre-test before the intervention started, and two post-
tests, one immediately
after the teaching had been completed and one about 2–4 weeks
after the teaching had
been completed.
At pre-test, they were
given 12 multiplicative reasoning problems (6 multiplication
and 6 division
problems), 6 additive reasoning problems, and the matrices subtest of
the
BAS-II (Elliott, Smith,
& McCulloch, 1997), which assesses children’s visual analytical
skills. In this subtest
of the BAS, the children are presented with an array of figures
displayed in a matrix
(2 £ 2 or 3 £ 3). The figure at the right-bottom corner is
missing.
The child is asked to
choose the correct one to complete the matrix, from six
alternatives. The
missing figure is predictable from the pattern: e.g. three crosses
in the
first column, three
black circles in the second column, and two white triangles in the
third column; the
bottom figure in this column is missing.
The children were given
the same measures of additive and multiplicative reasoning
in both post-tests as
in the pre-test. Our aim in the analysis of the additive reasoning
problems was twofold:
(1) to test whether the children in the experimental group had
learned correspondence
procedures in a mechanical and unreflective way or whether
they analysed the
relations in the problems before implementing the solution; and (2)
to
test whether they would
improve more than the children in the control group, who did
not solve numerical
problems during the intervention, in the additive reasoning
problems as a
consequence of having developed their general numerical skills. If
they
had learned to use
correspondences mechanically, they should show a decline in
performance in additive
reasoning problems by the inappropriate use of one-to-many
correspondence
procedures. If they developed their general numerical skills through
the intervention, they
should show some progress in additive reasoning.
Procedure
Both interventions
consisted of two teaching sessions, administered individually by a
researcher. The
children in the intervention group solved five multiplication and
five
division problems in
each session. The researcher asked the child to construct first a
representation of the
situation, using manipulatives to signify the objects mentioned
in the problems; if the
child did not spontaneously set the manipulatives in the
appropriate form of
one-to-many correspondence, the researcher encouraged the
child to do so. For
example, one multiplication problem was: three lorries are
bringing tables to the
school; each lorry is carrying four tables; how many tables are
they bringing to
school? The children were given cut-out figures of lorries and some
cubes, and were
encouraged to show what the problem had indicated. They received
further directives, as
required: for example, if the child made the correspondence for
one lorry and stopped,
the experimenter suggested showing the correspondence
for the other lorries
before answering the question. For division problems, the same
strategy was to be
implemented but the question was not about the total: e.g. a boy
has 12 marbles; he
wants to put the same number inside each of these two bags;
how many marbles will
go in each bag? The children had cut out circles to represent
the bags and cubes to
represent the marbles. The use of these simple manipulatives
to represent different
objects at different times during the sessions did not cause any
difficulty for the
children.
The children in the
control group worked individually with the researcher and
solved visual analysis
problems for a comparable period (approximately 25–30 min for
each session).
Results
The mean scores and
standard deviations for the children in each group at pre-test and
both post-tests are
presented in Table 2. The pre-test means show that the children in
this study had a much
weaker performance in the multiplicative reasoning problems
than those in Study 1.
They were, on average, 1 year younger than those in Study 1, and
also younger than US
1st graders (whose average age seems to be above 6 years).
Table 2. Means (out of
12) on the multiplicative reasoning tasks and standard deviations in
the pre- and
post-tests by group
Testing occasion
Intervention group
Mean
SD
N
Multiplicative
reasoning (max 12)
Pre-test
Control
Intervention
Immediate post-test
Control
Intervention
Delayed post-test
Control
Intervention
Additive reasoning (max
6)
Pre-test
Control
Intervention
Immediate post-test
Control
Intervention
Delayed post-test
Control
Intervention
1.93
2.23
2.73
7.35
3.00
7.76
3.07
3.18
2.93
4.82
3.47
4.53
1.75
2.11
2.43
2.74
3.27
3.99
1.75
1.78
1.83
0.81
1.64
1.42
15
17
15
17
15
17
15
17
15
17
15
17
Table 2 also shows that
the group that received instruction on correspondences
improved their
performance considerably in the multiplicative reasoning problems: at
pre-test, 19% of their
responses were correct and at the immediate post-test it was 61%,
an improvement of 42%.
The intervention group also improved on the additive
reasoning problems: at
pre-test, the rate of correct responses was 53% and at post-test
80%, an improvement of
27%. This indicates that they did not simply use
correspondence
procedures after the intervention in an unreflective manner.
As Table 2 also
shows, there was a difference between the groups in multiplicative
reasoning scores at
pre-test. This is a risk in random assignment to treatment groups
when
the number of
participants is small. So we carried out an analysis of covariance to
compare the performance
of the children in the two groups in the post-tests with the pre-
test scores as a
covariate. The means on the two post-tests, immediate and delayed,
were
the repeated measures
in this analysis. The group membership, control versus
experimental, was the
independent variable. This analysis showed that the overall effect
of testing occasion was
not significant. The effect of the covariate was significant
(F 1;29 ¼ 15:07, p ¼
:001). The difference between intervention groups was significant
(F 1;29 ¼ 8:49, p ¼
:007) and the interaction between testing occasion and intervention
group was also
significant (F 2;28 ¼ 8:87, p ¼ :006). Post hoc tests showed that
the
difference between the
groups was significant at both post-test occasions. Cohen’s d
effect size was
calculated using the adjusted means and the pooled standard deviation
for
the two groups on each
testing occasion. The effect size for the immediate post-test was
1.26 and for the
delayed post-test 1.03. So we can conclude that the intervention
group
improved significantly
more than the control group in multiplicative reasoning problems,
and a large effect size
was observed despite the fact that the intervention was so brief.
In order to test
whether children improve in problem solving from general
experiences in solving
problems, even if they do not involve the same type of relations,
we compared the
intervention’s and the control group’s improvement in additive
reasoning problems. We
used an analysis of covariance, in which the covariate was the
children’s pre-test
performance, to compare the intervention and control groups’
performance in additive
reasoning problems. The immediate and delayed post-tests
were treated as
repeated measures; the covariate was the pre-test measure; and the
dependent variable was
the number of correct responses to the additive reasoning
problems. The covariate
was significant (F 1;29 ¼ 6:83, p ¼ :014), the differences
between testing
occasion were significant (F 1;29 ¼ 9:14, p ¼ :005) and the
difference
between the groups was
also significant (F 1;29 ¼ 13:24, p ¼ :001), as predicted. Cohen’s
d was 1.01 for the
immediate post-test and 0.67 on the delayed post-test. These results
support the hypothesis
that children can improve their general numerical problem-
solving skills through
the experience of representing quantities numerically in order to
solve problems.
The same pattern of
specific connections between the tasks observed in Study 1,
which were analysed
through partial correlations, was observed in this study. The partial
correlations within
tasks were significant and those between tasks were not. The partial
correlation between
pre- and immediate post-test addition scores, controlling for
performance on the
pre-test multiplication task was significant (r ¼ :41, p ¼ :01)
and
the partial correlation
between pre- and immediate post-test multiplication scores,
controlling for
performance on the pre-test addition task was significant (r ¼ :50,
p ¼ :003). In
contrast, the partial correlation between pre-test addition scores
and post-
test multiplication
scores, controlling for pre-test multiplication scores was not
significant (r ¼ :30,
p . :05) and the partial correlation between pre-test multiplication
and post-test addition
scores, controlling for pre-test addition scores, was not significant
(r ¼ :08, p . :05).
The replication of the partial correlations in this study gives
further
support to the idea
that additive reasoning and multiplicative reasoning tasks are
distinct, apart from a
shared variance that can be attributed to general numerical skills in
problem solving.
In order to assess
whether the children in the control group had simply not been
interested in
participation in the study, we compared their results in the BAS-II
matrices
subtest with those
obtained by the experimental group on the three testing occasions.
We used an analysis of
covariance parallel to those described earlier on, with BAS-II
matrices subtest at
pre-test as a covariate and both post-tests as repeated measures. The
group membership was
the independent measure. This analysis showed a significant
effect of group
membership (F 1;29 ¼ 13:85, p ¼ :001); the control group performed
significantly better
than the intervention group in the control task, the BAS-II matrices
subtest, on both
post-tests but not at pre-test. We conclude that the control group
must
have remained motivated
throughout the experiment and gained significantly from their
experience in the
cognitive skill that they had practised during the study.
In summary, both groups
in this study made gains during the intervention.
These gains were
clearly related to the learning experiences that they had during the
teaching period.
We conclude that it is
possible to teach young children to use one-to-many
correspondence
reasoning to solve multiplicative reasoning problems. The 4- and
5-year-olds that
participated in this study showed clear gains in their general
numerical
skills and also in
their ability to solve multiplicative reasoning problems long before
they
would receive teaching
about multiplication and division in school. The children were
actually quite young
and their knowledge of addition and subtraction was still
developing, thus their
ability to solve multiplicative reasoning problems would be
developing at the same
time as their understanding of additive reasoning.
The importance of the
correspondence scheme for teaching and learning
Adopting one of
Piaget’s main hypotheses (see, for example, Piaget, 1950, 1952a,b),
we
have argued elsewhere
that the origin of children’s understanding of key mathematical
ideas is in their
action schemes (Nunes & Bryant, 1996) and have provided both
longitudinal and
intervention data to show a causal relation between children’s use
of
action schemes to solve
mathematical problems at school entry and their later mathematics
achievement (Nunes et
al., 2007). One-to-many correspondence was one of the action
schemes included in our
measure of children’s logical–mathematical reasoning.
In this previous study,
we did not report the results of the longitudinal prediction
obtained from the
children’s performance in the correspondence items and so we
report this result
here. The outcome measure in our previous study was the children’s
performance in a
government-designed and teacher-administered assessment of
mathematics
achievement, which is obtained at the end of their second year in
school.
The predictors were the
children’s age at the time of the achievement test and several
measures obtained at
school entry: (1) their general cognitive ability as measured by the
BAS-II, (2) working
memory, assessed by means of counting recall, a subtest from the
working memory battery
for children (Pickering & Gathercole, 2001), and our
assessment of their
logico-mathematical reasoning, which included their understanding
of the inverse relation
between addition and subtraction, one-to-many correspondences,
additive composition of
number and seriation.
In order to address the
significance of children’s ability to use correspondences to
solve multiplicative
reasoning problems as a predictor, excluding the other items in our
measure from their
score, we analysed how well the correspondence items predicted
the children’s
mathematics achievement. There were 12 items that involved
correspondences and we
obtained for all the children a score based only on these
12 items. We then ran a
fixed order regression to test whether these items still added
significantly to the
prediction of the children’s mathematics achievement, after
controlling for their
age, BAS-II and working memory performance. This analysis
showed that, after
controlling for the other predictors, the children’s performance
still
added significantly to
the prediction of their mathematics achievement (F 1;46 ¼ 5:05,
p , :05). The beta
values showed that this was not a minor contribution; the largest
beta
corresponded to the
BAS-II scores (b ¼ 0:4, p , :001), followed by the value for the
children’s use of
correspondences to solve multiplicative reasoning problems (b ¼ 0:3,
p ¼ :03); the
remaining beta values were not significant.
This overview of the
evidence on the role of correspondence reasoning for
children’s
mathematics learning shows that this is indeed a key understanding
for children’s
progress in mathematics. The use of correspondence reasoning allows
children to maintain a
fixed ratio between two variables, and thus use a type of
reasoning that differs
from part–whole reasoning and can form the basis for
understanding ratio and
proportions.
Our current hypothesis
is that the representation of ratio – i.e. of the relation
between the quantities
in these situations – remains implicit. It is likely that the
children
focus their attention
on the quantities themselves in such problems and they may not
build an awareness of
the relations between quantities. This would be entirely justified
by the types of
questions that they are asked ‘how many’, which are about
quantities.
For example, in
multiplication situations they are asked about the total number of
items
(e.g. in the problem
mentioned earlier on, they are asked how many tables were brought
to the school) and in
division situations they are asked about the quantities in a group
(e.g. how many marbles
in each bag) or about the number of groups.
Children often have
some implicit knowledge, which they develop outside school and
which they can use in
order to solve school problems. It is also quite commonly the case
that teachers need to
develop children’s awareness of this implicit knowledge in order to
teach them something
new. For some time now it has been recognized (Bryant & Bradley,
1985; Nunes &
Bryant, 2009) that children have a good knowledge of the sounds of
their own language,
which they use to speak and to discriminate between words that they
hear. However, this
knowledge is implicit and must be rendered explicit to some extent
in order for them to
learn to represent sounds using letters and to become literate.
We suggest that the
same sort of approach may be needed for children to transform
their implicit
understanding of fixed ratios, used in one-to-many correspondence
action
schemes, into an
understanding of ratio and proportions. This would allow them to
become conscious of the
relations that they only deal with implicitly, and help them
build a conception of
how relations between quantities are connected to the
mathematics that they
use in solving problems. Further research is urgently needed to
test this idea, which
may be very fruitful and help bring more success in mathematics to
many children in
school.
Nenhum comentário:
Postar um comentário